domingo, marzo 04, 2007

N.Y. Times Magazine

March 4, 2007

Darwin’s God

God has always been a puzzle for Scott Atran. When he was 10 years old, he scrawled a plaintive message on the wall of his bedroom in Baltimore. “God exists,” he wrote in black and orange paint, “or if he doesn’t, we’re in trouble.” Atran has been struggling with questions about religion ever since — why he himself no longer believes in God and why so many other people, everywhere in the world, apparently do.

Call it God; call it superstition; call it, as Atran does, “belief in hope beyond reason” — whatever you call it, there seems an inherent human drive to believe in something transcendent, unfathomable and otherworldly, something beyond the reach or understanding of science. “Why do we cross our fingers during turbulence, even the most atheistic among us?” asked Atran when we spoke at his Upper West Side pied-à-terre in January. Atran, who is 55, is an anthropologist at the National Center for Scientific Research in Paris, with joint appointments at the University of Michigan and the John Jay College of Criminal Justice in New York. His research interests include cognitive science and evolutionary biology, and sometimes he presents students with a wooden box that he pretends is an African relic. “If you have negative sentiments toward religion,” he tells them, “the box will destroy whatever you put inside it.” Many of his students say they doubt the existence of God, but in this demonstration they act as if they believe in something. Put your pencil into the magic box, he tells them, and the nonbelievers do so blithely. Put in your driver’s license, he says, and most do, but only after significant hesitation. And when he tells them to put in their hands, few will.

If they don’t believe in God, what exactly are they afraid of?

Atran first conducted the magic-box demonstration in the 1980s, when he was at Cambridge University studying the nature of religious belief. He had received a doctorate in anthropology from Columbia University and, in the course of his fieldwork, saw evidence of religion everywhere he looked — at archaeological digs in Israel, among the Mayans in Guatemala, in artifact drawers at the American Museum of Natural History in New York. Atran is Darwinian in his approach, which means he tries to explain behavior by how it might once have solved problems of survival and reproduction for our early ancestors. But it was not clear to him what evolutionary problems might have been solved by religious belief. Religion seemed to use up physical and mental resources without an obvious benefit for survival. Why, he wondered, was religion so pervasive, when it was something that seemed so costly from an evolutionary point of view?

The magic-box demonstration helped set Atran on a career studying why humans might have evolved to be religious, something few people were doing back in the ’80s. Today, the effort has gained momentum, as scientists search for an evolutionary explanation for why belief in God exists — not whether God exists, which is a matter for philosophers and theologians, but why the belief does.

This is different from the scientific assault on religion that has been garnering attention recently, in the form of best-selling books from scientific atheists who see religion as a scourge. In “The God Delusion,” published last year and still on best-seller lists, the Oxford evolutionary biologist Richard Dawkins concludes that religion is nothing more than a useless, and sometimes dangerous, evolutionary accident. “Religious behavior may be a misfiring, an unfortunate byproduct of an underlying psychological propensity which in other circumstances is, or once was, useful,” Dawkins wrote. He is joined by two other best-selling authors — Sam Harris, who wrote “The End of Faith,” and Daniel Dennett, a philosopher at Tufts University who wrote “Breaking the Spell.” The three men differ in their personal styles and whether they are engaged in a battle against religiosity, but their names are often mentioned together. They have been portrayed as an unholy trinity of neo-atheists, promoting their secular world view with a fervor that seems almost evangelical.

Lost in the hullabaloo over the neo-atheists is a quieter and potentially more illuminating debate. It is taking place not between science and religion but within science itself, specifically among the scientists studying the evolution of religion. These scholars tend to agree on one point: that religious belief is an outgrowth of brain architecture that evolved during early human history. What they disagree about is why a tendency to believe evolved, whether it was because belief itself was adaptive or because it was just an evolutionary byproduct, a mere consequence of some other adaptation in the evolution of the human brain.

Which is the better biological explanation for a belief in God — evolutionary adaptation or neurological accident? Is there something about the cognitive functioning of humans that makes us receptive to belief in a supernatural deity? And if scientists are able to explain God, what then? Is explaining religion the same thing as explaining it away? Are the nonbelievers right, and is religion at its core an empty undertaking, a misdirection, a vestigial artifact of a primitive mind? Or are the believers right, and does the fact that we have the mental capacities for discerning God suggest that it was God who put them there?

In short, are we hard-wired to believe in God? And if we are, how and why did that happen?

“All of our raptures and our drynesses, our longings and pantings, our questions and beliefs . . . are equally organically founded,” William James wrote in “The Varieties of Religious Experience.” James, who taught philosophy and experimental psychology at Harvard for more than 30 years, based his book on a 1901 lecture series in which he took some early tentative steps at breaching the science-religion divide.

In the century that followed, a polite convention generally separated science and religion, at least in much of the Western world. Science, as the old trope had it, was assigned the territory that describes how the heavens go; religion, how to go to heaven.

Anthropologists like Atran and psychologists as far back as James had been looking at the roots of religion, but the mutual hands-off policy really began to shift in the 1990s. Religion made incursions into the traditional domain of science with attempts to bring intelligent design into the biology classroom and to choke off human embryonic stem-cell research on religious grounds. Scientists responded with counterincursions. Experts from the hard sciences, like evolutionary biology and cognitive neuroscience, joined anthropologists and psychologists in the study of religion, making God an object of scientific inquiry.

The debate over why belief evolved is between byproduct theorists and adaptationists. You might think that the byproduct theorists would tend to be nonbelievers, looking for a way to explain religion as a fluke, while the adaptationists would be more likely to be believers who can intuit the emotional, spiritual and community advantages that accompany faith. Or you might think they would all be atheists, because what believer would want to subject his own devotion to rationalism’s cold, hard scrutiny? But a scientist’s personal religious view does not always predict which side he will take. And this is just one sign of how complex and surprising this debate has become.

Angels, demons, spirits, wizards, gods and witches have peppered folk religions since mankind first started telling stories. Charles Darwin noted this in “The Descent of Man.” “A belief in all-pervading spiritual agencies,” he wrote, “seems to be universal.” According to anthropologists, religions that share certain supernatural features — belief in a noncorporeal God or gods, belief in the afterlife, belief in the ability of prayer or ritual to change the course of human events — are found in virtually every culture on earth.

This is certainly true in the United States. About 6 in 10 Americans, according to a 2005 Harris Poll, believe in the devil and hell, and about 7 in 10 believe in angels, heaven and the existence of miracles and of life after death. A 2006 survey at Baylor University found that 92 percent of respondents believe in a personal God — that is, a God with a distinct set of character traits ranging from “distant” to “benevolent.”

When a trait is universal, evolutionary biologists look for a genetic explanation and wonder how that gene or genes might enhance survival or reproductive success. In many ways, it’s an exercise in post-hoc hypothesizing: what would have been the advantage, when the human species first evolved, for an individual who happened to have a mutation that led to, say, a smaller jaw, a bigger forehead, a better thumb? How about certain behavioral traits, like a tendency for risk-taking or for kindness?

Atran saw such questions as a puzzle when applied to religion. So many aspects of religious belief involve misattribution and misunderstanding of the real world. Wouldn’t this be a liability in the survival-of-the-fittest competition? To Atran, religious belief requires taking “what is materially false to be true” and “what is materially true to be false.” One example of this is the belief that even after someone dies and the body demonstrably disintegrates, that person will still exist, will still be able to laugh and cry, to feel pain and joy. This confusion “does not appear to be a reasonable evolutionary strategy,” Atran wrote in “In Gods We Trust: The Evolutionary Landscape of Religion” in 2002. “Imagine another animal that took injury for health or big for small or fast for slow or dead for alive. It’s unlikely that such a species could survive.” He began to look for a sideways explanation: if religious belief was not adaptive, perhaps it was associated with something else that was.

Atran intended to study mathematics when he entered Columbia as a precocious 17-year-old. But he was distracted by the radical politics of the late ’60s. One day in his freshman year, he found himself at an antiwar rally listening to Margaret Mead, then perhaps the most famous anthropologist in America. Atran, dressed in a flamboyant Uncle Sam suit, stood up and called her a sellout for saying the protesters should be writing to their congressmen instead of staging demonstrations. “Young man,” the unflappable Mead said, “why don’t you come see me in my office?”

Atran, equally unflappable, did go to see her — and ended up working for Mead, spending much of his time exploring the cabinets of curiosities in her tower office at the American Museum of Natural History. Soon he switched his major to anthropology.

Many of the museum specimens were religious, Atran says. So were the artifacts he dug up on archaeological excursions in Israel in the early ’70s. Wherever he turned, he encountered the passion of religious belief. Why, he wondered, did people work so hard against their preference for logical explanations to maintain two views of the world, the real and the unreal, the intuitive and the counterintuitive?

Maybe cognitive effort was precisely the point. Maybe it took less mental work than Atran realized to hold belief in God in one’s mind. Maybe, in fact, belief was the default position for the human mind, something that took no cognitive effort at all.

While still an undergraduate, Atran decided to explore these questions by organizing a conference on universal aspects of culture and inviting all his intellectual heroes: the linguist Noam Chomsky, the psychologist Jean Piaget, the anthropologists Claude Levi-Strauss and Gregory Bateson (who was also Margaret Mead’s ex-husband), the Nobel Prize-winning biologists Jacques Monod and Francois Jacob. It was 1974, and the only site he could find for the conference was at a location just outside Paris. Atran was a scraggly 22-year-old with a guitar who had learned his French from comic books. To his astonishment, everyone he invited agreed to come.

Atran is a sociable man with sharp hazel eyes, who sparks provocative conversations the way other men pick bar fights. As he traveled in the ’70s and ’80s, he accumulated friends who were thinking about the issues he was: how culture is transmitted among human groups and what evolutionary function it might serve. “I started looking at history, and I wondered why no society ever survived more than three generations without a religious foundation as its raison d’être,” he says. Soon he turned to an emerging subset of evolutionary theory — the evolution of human cognition.

Some cognitive scientists think of brain functioning in terms of modules, a series of interconnected machines, each one responsible for a particular mental trick. They do not tend to talk about a God module per se; they usually consider belief in God a consequence of other mental modules.

Religion, in this view, is “a family of cognitive phenomena that involves the extraordinary use of everyday cognitive processes,” Atran wrote in “In Gods We Trust.” “Religions do not exist apart from the individual minds that constitute them and the environments that constrain them, any more than biological species and varieties exist independently of the individual organisms that compose them and the environments that conform them.”

At around the time “In Gods We Trust” appeared five years ago, a handful of other scientists — Pascal Boyer, now at Washington University; Justin Barrett, now at Oxford; Paul Bloom at Yale — were addressing these same questions. In synchrony they were moving toward the byproduct theory.

Darwinians who study physical evolution distinguish between traits that are themselves adaptive, like having blood cells that can transport oxygen, and traits that are byproducts of adaptations, like the redness of blood. There is no survival advantage to blood’s being red instead of turquoise; it is just a byproduct of the trait that is adaptive, having blood that contains hemoglobin.

Something similar explains aspects of brain evolution, too, say the byproduct theorists. Which brings us to the idea of the spandrel.

Stephen Jay Gould, the famed evolutionary biologist at Harvard who died in 2002, and his colleague Richard Lewontin proposed “spandrel” to describe a trait that has no adaptive value of its own. They borrowed the term from architecture, where it originally referred to the V-shaped structure formed between two rounded arches. The structure is not there for any purpose; it is there because that is what happens when arches align.

In architecture, a spandrel can be neutral or it can be made functional. Building a staircase, for instance, creates a space underneath that is innocuous, just a blank sort of triangle. But if you put a closet there, the under-stairs space takes on a function, unrelated to the staircase’s but useful nonetheless. Either way, functional or nonfunctional, the space under the stairs is a spandrel, an unintended byproduct.

“Natural selection made the human brain big,” Gould wrote, “but most of our mental properties and potentials may be spandrels — that is, nonadaptive side consequences of building a device with such structural complexity.”

The possibility that God could be a spandrel offered Atran a new way of understanding the evolution of religion. But a spandrel of what, exactly?

Hardships of early human life favored the evolution of certain cognitive tools, among them the ability to infer the presence of organisms that might do harm, to come up with causal narratives for natural events and to recognize that other people have minds of their own with their own beliefs, desires and intentions. Psychologists call these tools, respectively, agent detection, causal reasoning and theory of mind.

Agent detection evolved because assuming the presence of an agent — which is jargon for any creature with volitional, independent behavior — is more adaptive than assuming its absence. If you are a caveman on the savannah, you are better off presuming that the motion you detect out of the corner of your eye is an agent and something to run from, even if you are wrong. If it turns out to have been just the rustling of leaves, you are still alive; if what you took to be leaves rustling was really a hyena about to pounce, you are dead.

A classic experiment from the 1940s by the psychologists Fritz Heider and Marianne Simmel suggested that imputing agency is so automatic that people may do it even for geometric shapes. For the experiment, subjects watched a film of triangles and circles moving around. When asked what they had been watching, the subjects used words like “chase” and “capture.” They did not just see the random movement of shapes on a screen; they saw pursuit, planning, escape.

So if there is motion just out of our line of sight, we presume it is caused by an agent, an animal or person with the ability to move independently. This usually operates in one direction only; lots of people mistake a rock for a bear, but almost no one mistakes a bear for a rock.

What does this mean for belief in the supernatural? It means our brains are primed for it, ready to presume the presence of agents even when such presence confounds logic. “The most central concepts in religions are related to agents,” Justin Barrett, a psychologist, wrote in his 2004 summary of the byproduct theory, “Why Would Anyone Believe in God?” Religious agents are often supernatural, he wrote, “people with superpowers, statues that can answer requests or disembodied minds that can act on us and the world.”

A second mental module that primes us for religion is causal reasoning. The human brain has evolved the capacity to impose a narrative, complete with chronology and cause-and-effect logic, on whatever it encounters, no matter how apparently random. “We automatically, and often unconsciously, look for an explanation of why things happen to us,” Barrett wrote, “and ‘stuff just happens’ is no explanation. Gods, by virtue of their strange physical properties and their mysterious superpowers, make fine candidates for causes of many of these unusual events.” The ancient Greeks believed thunder was the sound of Zeus’s thunderbolt. Similarly, a contemporary woman whose cancer treatment works despite 10-to-1 odds might look for a story to explain her survival. It fits better with her causal-reasoning tool for her recovery to be a miracle, or a reward for prayer, than for it to be just a lucky roll of the dice.

A third cognitive trick is a kind of social intuition known as theory of mind. It’s an odd phrase for something so automatic, since the word “theory” suggests formality and self-consciousness. Other terms have been used for the same concept, like intentional stance and social cognition. One good alternative is the term Atran uses: folkpsychology.

Folkpsychology, as Atran and his colleagues see it, is essential to getting along in the contemporary world, just as it has been since prehistoric times. It allows us to anticipate the actions of others and to lead others to believe what we want them to believe; it is at the heart of everything from marriage to office politics to poker. People without this trait, like those with severe autism, are impaired, unable to imagine themselves in other people’s heads.

The process begins with positing the existence of minds, our own and others’, that we cannot see or feel. This leaves us open, almost instinctively, to belief in the separation of the body (the visible) and the mind (the invisible). If you can posit minds in other people that you cannot verify empirically, suggests Paul Bloom, a psychologist and the author of “Descartes’ Baby,” published in 2004, it is a short step to positing minds that do not have to be anchored to a body. And from there, he said, it is another short step to positing an immaterial soul and a transcendent God.

The traditional psychological view has been that until about age 4, children think that minds are permeable and that everyone knows whatever the child himself knows. To a young child, everyone is infallible. All other people, especially Mother and Father, are thought to have the same sort of insight as an all-knowing God.

But at a certain point in development, this changes. (Some new research suggests this might occur as early as 15 months.) The “false-belief test” is a classic experiment that highlights the boundary. Children watch a puppet show with a simple plot: John comes onstage holding a marble, puts it in Box A and walks off. Mary comes onstage, opens Box A, takes out the marble, puts it in Box B and walks off. John comes back onstage. The children are asked, Where will John look for the marble?

Very young children, or autistic children of any age, say John will look in Box B, since they know that’s where the marble is. But older children give a more sophisticated answer. They know that John never saw Mary move the marble and that as far as he is concerned it is still where he put it, in Box A. Older children have developed a theory of mind; they understand that other people sometimes have false beliefs. Even though they know that the marble is in Box B, they respond that John will look for it in Box A.

The adaptive advantage of folkpsychology is obvious. According to Atran, our ancestors needed it to survive their harsh environment, since folkpsychology allowed them to “rapidly and economically” distinguish good guys from bad guys. But how did folkpsychology — an understanding of ordinary people’s ordinary minds — allow for a belief in supernatural, omniscient minds? And if the byproduct theorists are right and these beliefs were of little use in finding food or leaving more offspring, why did they persist?

Atran ascribes the persistence to evolutionary misdirection, which, he says, happens all the time: “Evolution always produces something that works for what it works for, and then there’s no control for however else it’s used.” On a sunny weekday morning, over breakfast at a French cafe on upper Broadway, he tried to think of an analogy and grinned when he came up with an old standby: women’s breasts. Because they are associated with female hormones, he explained, full breasts indicate a woman is fertile, and the evolution of the male brain’s preference for them was a clever mating strategy. But breasts are now used for purposes unrelated to reproduction, to sell anything from deodorant to beer. “A Martian anthropologist might look at this and say, ‘Oh, yes, so these breasts must have somehow evolved to sell hygienic stuff or food to human beings,’ ” Atran said. But the Martian would, of course, be wrong. Equally wrong would be to make the same mistake about religion, thinking it must have evolved to make people behave a certain way or feel a certain allegiance.

That is what most fascinated Atran. “Why is God in there?” he wondered.

The idea of an infallible God is comfortable and familiar, something children readily accept. You can see this in the experiment Justin Barrett conducted recently — a version of the traditional false-belief test but with a religious twist. Barrett showed young children a box with a picture of crackers on the outside. What do you think is inside this box? he asked, and the children said, “Crackers.” Next he opened it and showed them that the box was filled with rocks. Then he asked two follow-up questions: What would your mother say is inside this box? And what would God say?

As earlier theory-of-mind experiments already showed, 3- and 4-year-olds tended to think Mother was infallible, and since the children knew the right answer, they assumed she would know it, too. They usually responded that Mother would say the box contained rocks. But 5- and 6-year-olds had learned that Mother, like any other person, could hold a false belief in her mind, and they tended to respond that she would be fooled by the packaging and would say, “Crackers.”

And what would God say? No matter what their age, the children, who were all Protestants, told Barrett that God would answer, “Rocks.” This was true even for the older children, who, as Barrett understood it, had developed folkpsychology and had used it when predicting a wrong response for Mother. They had learned that, in certain situations, people could be fooled — but they had also learned that there is no fooling God.

The bottom line, according to byproduct theorists, is that children are born with a tendency to believe in omniscience, invisible minds, immaterial souls — and then they grow up in cultures that fill their minds, hard-wired for belief, with specifics. It is a little like language acquisition, Paul Bloom says, with the essential difference that language is a biological adaptation and religion, in his view, is not. We are born with an innate facility for language but the specific language we learn depends on the environment in which we are raised. In much the same way, he says, we are born with an innate tendency for belief, but the specifics of what we grow up believing — whether there is one God or many, whether the soul goes to heaven or occupies another animal after death — are culturally shaped.

Whatever the specifics, certain beliefs can be found in all religions. Those that prevail, according to the byproduct theorists, are those that fit most comfortably with our mental architecture. Psychologists have shown, for instance, that people attend to, and remember, things that are unfamiliar and strange, but not so strange as to be impossible to assimilate. Ideas about God or other supernatural agents tend to fit these criteria. They are what Pascal Boyer, an anthropologist and psychologist, called “minimally counterintuitive”: weird enough to get your attention and lodge in your memory but not so weird that you reject them altogether. A tree that talks is minimally counterintuitive, and you might believe it as a supernatural agent. A tree that talks and flies and time-travels is maximally counterintuitive, and you are more likely to reject it.

Atran, along with Ara Norenzayan of the University of British Columbia, studied the idea of minimally counterintuitive agents earlier this decade. They presented college students with lists of fantastical creatures and asked them to choose the ones that seemed most “religious.” The convincingly religious agents, the students said, were not the most outlandish — not the turtle that chatters and climbs or the squealing, flowering marble — but those that were just outlandish enough: giggling seaweed, a sobbing oak, a talking horse. Giggling seaweed meets the requirement of being minimally counterintuitive, Atran wrote. So does a God who has a human personality except that he knows everything or a God who has a mind but has no body.

It is not enough for an agent to be minimally counterintuitive for it to earn a spot in people’s belief systems. An emotional component is often needed, too, if belief is to take hold. “If your emotions are involved, then that’s the time when you’re most likely to believe whatever the religion tells you to believe,” Atran says. Religions stir up emotions through their rituals — swaying, singing, bowing in unison during group prayer, sometimes working people up to a state of physical arousal that can border on frenzy. And religions gain strength during the natural heightening of emotions that occurs in times of personal crisis, when the faithful often turn to shamans or priests. The most intense personal crisis, for which religion can offer powerfully comforting answers, is when someone comes face to face with mortality.

In John Updike’s celebrated early short story “Pigeon Feathers,” 14-year-old David spends a lot of time thinking about death. He suspects that adults are lying when they say his spirit will live on after he dies. He keeps catching them in inconsistencies when he asks where exactly his soul will spend eternity. “Don’t you see,” he cries to his mother, “if when we die there’s nothing, all your sun and fields and what not are all, ah, horror? It’s just an ocean of horror.”

The story ends with David’s tiny revelation and his boundless relief. The boy gets a gun for his 15th birthday, which he uses to shoot down some pigeons that have been nesting in his grandmother’s barn. Before he buries them, he studies the dead birds’ feathers. He is amazed by their swirls of color, “designs executed, it seemed, in a controlled rapture.” And suddenly the fears that have plagued him are lifted, and with a “slipping sensation along his nerves that seemed to give the air hands, he was robed in this certainty: that the God who had lavished such craft upon these worthless birds would not destroy His whole Creation by refusing to let David live forever.”

Fear of death is an undercurrent of belief. The spirits of dead ancestors, ghosts, immortal deities, heaven and hell, the everlasting soul: the notion of spiritual existence after death is at the heart of almost every religion. According to some adaptationists, this is part of religion’s role, to help humans deal with the grim certainty of death. Believing in God and the afterlife, they say, is how we make sense of the brevity of our time on earth, how we give meaning to this brutish and short existence. Religion can offer solace to the bereaved and comfort to the frightened.

But the spandrelists counter that saying these beliefs are consolation does not mean they offered an adaptive advantage to our ancestors. “The human mind does not produce adequate comforting delusions against all situations of stress or fear,” wrote Pascal Boyer, a leading byproduct theorist, in “Religion Explained,” which came out a year before Atran’s book. “Indeed, any organism that was prone to such delusions would not survive long.”

Whether or not it is adaptive, belief in the afterlife gains power in two ways: from the intensity with which people wish it to be true and from the confirmation it seems to get from the real world. This brings us back to folkpsychology. We try to make sense of other people partly by imagining what it is like to be them, an adaptive trait that allowed our ancestors to outwit potential enemies. But when we think about being dead, we run into a cognitive wall. How can we possibly think about not thinking? “Try to fill your consciousness with the representation of no-consciousness, and you will see the impossibility of it,” the Spanish philosopher Miguel de Unamuno wrote in “Tragic Sense of Life.” “The effort to comprehend it causes the most tormenting dizziness. We cannot conceive of ourselves as not existing.”

Much easier, then, to imagine that the thinking somehow continues. This is what young children seem to do, as a study at the Florida Atlantic University demonstrated a few years ago. Jesse Bering and David Bjorklund, the psychologists who conducted the study, used finger puppets to act out the story of a mouse, hungry and lost, who is spotted by an alligator. “Well, it looks like Brown Mouse got eaten by Mr. Alligator,” the narrator says at the end. “Brown Mouse is not alive anymore.”

Afterward, Bering and Bjorklund asked their subjects, ages 4 to 12, what it meant for Brown Mouse to be “not alive anymore.” Is he still hungry? Is he still sleepy? Does he still want to go home? Most said the mouse no longer needed to eat or drink. But a large proportion, especially the younger ones, said that he still had thoughts, still loved his mother and still liked cheese. The children understood what it meant for the mouse’s body to cease to function, but many believed that something about the mouse was still alive.

“Our psychological architecture makes us think in particular ways,” says Bering, now at Queens University in Belfast, Northern Ireland. “In this study, it seems, the reason afterlife beliefs are so prevalent is that underlying them is our inability to simulate our nonexistence.”

It might be just as impossible to simulate the nonexistence of loved ones. A large part of any relationship takes place in our minds, Bering said, so it’s natural for it to continue much as before after the other person’s death. It is easy to forget that your sister is dead when you reach for the phone to call her, since your relationship was based so much on memory and imagined conversations even when she was alive. In addition, our agent-detection device sometimes confirms the sensation that the dead are still with us. The wind brushes our cheek, a spectral shape somehow looks familiar and our agent detection goes into overdrive. Dreams, too, have a way of confirming belief in the afterlife, with dead relatives appearing in dreams as if from beyond the grave, seeming very much alive.

Belief is our fallback position, according to Bering; it is our reflexive style of thought. “We have a basic psychological capacity that allows anyone to reason about unexpected natural events, to see deeper meaning where there is none,” he says. “It’s natural; it’s how our minds work.”

Intriguing as the spandrel logic might be, there is another way to think about the evolution of religion: that religion evolved because it offered survival advantages to our distant ancestors. This is where the action is in the science of God debate, with a coterie of adaptationists arguing on behalf of the primary benefits, in terms of survival advantages, of religious belief.

The trick in thinking about adaptation is that even if a trait offers no survival advantage today, it might have had one long ago. This is how Darwinians explain how certain physical characteristics persist even if they do not currently seem adaptive — by asking whether they might have helped our distant ancestors form social groups, feed themselves, find suitable mates or keep from getting killed. A facility for storing calories as fat, for instance, which is a detriment in today’s food-rich society, probably helped our ancestors survive cyclical famines.

So trying to explain the adaptiveness of religion means looking for how it might have helped early humans survive and reproduce. As some adaptationists see it, this could have worked on two levels, individual and group. Religion made people feel better, less tormented by thoughts about death, more focused on the future, more willing to take care of themselves. As William James put it, religion filled people with “a new zest which adds itself like a gift to life . . . an assurance of safety and a temper of peace and, in relation to others, a preponderance of loving affections.”

Such sentiments, some adaptationists say, made the faithful better at finding and storing food, for instance, and helped them attract better mates because of their reputations for morality, obedience and sober living. The advantage might have worked at the group level too, with religious groups outlasting others because they were more cohesive, more likely to contain individuals willing to make sacrifices for the group and more adept at sharing resources and preparing for warfare.

One of the most vocal adaptationists is David Sloan Wilson, an occasional thorn in the side of both Scott Atran and Richard Dawkins. Wilson, an evolutionary biologist at the State University of New York at Binghamton, focuses much of his argument at the group level. “Organisms are a product of natural selection,” he wrote in “Darwin’s Cathedral: Evolution, Religion, and the Nature of Society,” which came out in 2002, the same year as Atran’s book, and staked out the adaptationist view. “Through countless generations of variation and selection, [organisms] acquire properties that enable them to survive and reproduce in their environments. My purpose is to see if human groups in general, and religious groups in particular, qualify as organismic in this sense.”

Wilson’s father was Sloan Wilson, author of “The Man in the Gray Flannel Suit,” an emblem of mid-’50s suburban anomie that was turned into a film starring Gregory Peck. Sloan Wilson became a celebrity, with young women asking for his autograph, especially after his next novel, “A Summer Place,” became another blockbuster movie. The son grew up wanting to do something to make his famous father proud.

“I knew I couldn’t be a novelist,” said Wilson, who crackled with intensity during a telephone interview, “so I chose something as far as possible from literature — I chose science.” He is disarmingly honest about what motivated him: “I was very ambitious, and I wanted to make a mark.” He chose to study human evolution, he said, in part because he had some of his father’s literary leanings and the field required a novelist’s attention to human motivations, struggles and alliances — as well as a novelist’s flair for narrative.

Wilson eventually chose to study religion not because religion mattered to him personally — he was raised in a secular Protestant household and says he has long been an atheist — but because it was a lens through which to look at and revivify a branch of evolution that had fallen into disrepute. When Wilson was a graduate student at Michigan State University in the 1970s, Darwinians were critical of group selection, the idea that human groups can function as single organisms the way beehives or anthills do. So he decided to become the man who rescued this discredited idea. “I thought, Wow, defending group selection — now, that would be big,” he recalled. It wasn’t until the 1990s, he said, that he realized that “religion offered an opportunity to show that group selection was right after all.”

Dawkins once called Wilson’s defense of group selection “sheer, wanton, head-in-bag perversity.” Atran, too, has been dismissive of this approach, calling it “mind blind” for essentially ignoring the role of the brain’s mental machinery. The adaptationists “cannot in principle distinguish Marxism from monotheism, ideology from religious belief,” Atran wrote. “They cannot explain why people can be more steadfast in their commitment to admittedly counterfactual and counterintuitive beliefs — that Mary is both a mother and a virgin, and God is sentient but bodiless — than to the most politically, economically or scientifically persuasive account of the way things are or should be.”

Still, for all its controversial elements, the narrative Wilson devised about group selection and the evolution of religion is clear, perhaps a legacy of his novelist father. Begin, he says, with an imaginary flock of birds. Some birds serve as sentries, scanning the horizon for predators and calling out warnings. Having a sentry is good for the group but bad for the sentry, which is doubly harmed: by keeping watch, the sentry has less time to gather food, and by issuing a warning call, it is more likely to be spotted by the predator. So in the Darwinian struggle, the birds most likely to pass on their genes are the nonsentries. How, then, could the sentry gene survive for more than a generation or two?

To explain how a self-sacrificing gene can persist, Wilson looks to the level of the group. If there are 10 sentries in one group and none in the other, 3 or 4 of the sentries might be sacrificed. But the flock with sentries will probably outlast the flock that has no early-warning system, so the other 6 or 7 sentries will survive to pass on the genes. In other words, if the whole-group advantage outweighs the cost to any individual bird of being a sentry, then the sentry gene will prevail.

There are costs to any individual of being religious: the time and resources spent on rituals, the psychic energy devoted to following certain injunctions, the pain of some initiation rites. But in terms of intergroup struggle, according to Wilson, the costs can be outweighed by the benefits of being in a cohesive group that out-competes the others.

There is another element here too, unique to humans because it depends on language. A person’s behavior is observed not only by those in his immediate surroundings but also by anyone who can hear about it. There might be clear costs to taking on a role analogous to the sentry bird — a person who stands up to authority, for instance, risks losing his job, going to jail or getting beaten by the police — but in humans, these local costs might be outweighed by long-distance benefits. If a particular selfless trait enhances a person’s reputation, spread through the written and spoken word, it might give him an advantage in many of life’s challenges, like finding a mate. One way that reputation is enhanced is by being ostentatiously religious.

“The study of evolution is largely the study of trade-offs,” Wilson wrote in “Darwin’s Cathedral.” It might seem disadvantageous, in terms of foraging for sustenance and safety, for someone to favor religious over rationalistic explanations that would point to where the food and danger are. But in some circumstances, he wrote, “a symbolic belief system that departs from factual reality fares better.” For the individual, it might be more adaptive to have “highly sophisticated mental modules for acquiring factual knowledge and for building symbolic belief systems” than to have only one or the other, according to Wilson. For the group, it might be that a mixture of hardheaded realists and symbolically minded visionaries is most adaptive and that “what seems to be an adversarial relationship” between theists and atheists within a community is really a division of cognitive labor that “keeps social groups as a whole on an even keel.”

Even if Wilson is right that religion enhances group fitness, the question remains: Where does God come in? Why is a religious group any different from groups for which a fitness argument is never even offered — a group of fraternity brothers, say, or Yankees fans?

Richard Sosis, an anthropologist with positions at the University of Connecticut and Hebrew University of Jerusalem, has suggested a partial answer. Like many adaptationists, Sosis focuses on the way religion might be adaptive at the individual level. But even adaptations that help an individual survive can sometimes play themselves out through the group. Consider religious rituals.

“Religious and secular rituals can both promote cooperation,” Sosis wrote in American Scientist in 2004. But religious rituals “generate greater belief and commitment” because they depend on belief rather than on proof. The rituals are “beyond the possibility of examination,” he wrote, and a commitment to them is therefore emotional rather than logical — a commitment that is, in Sosis’s view, deeper and more long-lasting.

Rituals are a way of signaling a sincere commitment to the religion’s core beliefs, thereby earning loyalty from others in the group. “By donning several layers of clothing and standing out in the midday sun,” Sosis wrote, “ultraorthodox Jewish men are signaling to others: ‘Hey! Look, I’m a haredi’ — or extremely pious — ‘Jew. If you are also a member of this group, you can trust me because why else would I be dressed like this?’ ” These “signaling” rituals can grant the individual a sense of belonging and grant the group some freedom from constant and costly monitoring to ensure that their members are loyal and committed. The rituals are harsh enough to weed out the infidels, and both the group and the individual believers benefit.

In 2003, Sosis and Bradley Ruffle of Ben Gurion University in Israel sought an explanation for why Israel’s religious communes did better on average than secular communes in the wake of the economic crash of most of the country’s kibbutzim. They based their study on a standard economic game that measures cooperation. Individuals from religious communes played the game more cooperatively, while those from secular communes tended to be more selfish. It was the men who attended synagogue daily, not the religious women or the less observant men, who showed the biggest differences. To Sosis, this suggested that what mattered most was the frequent public display of devotion. These rituals, he wrote, led to greater cooperation in the religious communes, which helped them maintain their communal structure during economic hard times.

In 1997, Stephen Jay Gould wrote an essay in Natural History that called for a truce between religion and science. “The net of science covers the empirical universe,” he wrote. “The net of religion extends over questions of moral meaning and value.” Gould was emphatic about keeping the domains separate, urging “respectful discourse” and “mutual humility.” He called the demarcation “nonoverlapping magisteria” from the Latin magister, meaning “canon.”

Richard Dawkins had a history of spirited arguments with Gould, with whom he disagreed about almost everything related to the timing and focus of evolution. But he reserved some of his most venomous words for nonoverlapping magisteria. “Gould carried the art of bending over backward to positively supine lengths,” he wrote in “The God Delusion.” “Why shouldn’t we comment on God, as scientists? . . . A universe with a creative superintendent would be a very different kind of universe from one without. Why is that not a scientific matter?”

The separation, other critics said, left untapped the potential richness of letting one worldview inform the other. “Even if Gould was right that there were two domains, what religion does and what science does,” says Daniel Dennett (who, despite his neo-atheist label, is not as bluntly antireligious as Dawkins and Harris are), “that doesn’t mean science can’t study what religion does. It just means science can’t do what religion does.”

The idea that religion can be studied as a natural phenomenon might seem to require an atheistic philosophy as a starting point. Not necessarily. Even some neo-atheists aren’t entirely opposed to religion. Sam Harris practices Buddhist-inspired meditation. Daniel Dennett holds an annual Christmas sing-along, complete with hymns and carols that are not only harmonically lush but explicitly pious.

And one prominent member of the byproduct camp, Justin Barrett, is an observant Christian who believes in “an all-knowing, all-powerful, perfectly good God who brought the universe into being,” as he wrote in an e-mail message. “I believe that the purpose for people is to love God and love each other.”

At first blush, Barrett’s faith might seem confusing. How does his view of God as a byproduct of our mental architecture coexist with his Christianity? Why doesn’t the byproduct theory turn him into a skeptic?

“Christian theology teaches that people were crafted by God to be in a loving relationship with him and other people,” Barrett wrote in his e-mail message. “Why wouldn’t God, then, design us in such a way as to find belief in divinity quite natural?” Having a scientific explanation for mental phenomena does not mean we should stop believing in them, he wrote. “Suppose science produces a convincing account for why I think my wife loves me — should I then stop believing that she does?”

What can be made of atheists, then? If the evolutionary view of religion is true, they have to work hard at being atheists, to resist slipping into intrinsic habits of mind that make it easier to believe than not to believe. Atran says he faces an emotional and intellectual struggle to live without God in a nonatheist world, and he suspects that is where his little superstitions come from, his passing thought about crossing his fingers during turbulence or knocking on wood just in case. It is like an atavistic theism erupting when his guard is down. The comforts and consolations of belief are alluring even to him, he says, and probably will become more so as he gets closer to the end of his life. He fights it because he is a scientist and holds the values of rationalism higher than the values of spiritualism.

This internal push and pull between the spiritual and the rational reflects what used to be called the “God of the gaps” view of religion. The presumption was that as science was able to answer more questions about the natural world, God would be invoked to answer fewer, and religion would eventually recede. Research about the evolution of religion suggests otherwise. No matter how much science can explain, it seems, the real gap that God fills is an emptiness that our big-brained mental architecture interprets as a yearning for the supernatural. The drive to satisfy that yearning, according to both adaptationists and byproduct theorists, might be an inevitable and eternal part of what Atran calls the tragedy of human cognition.

Robin Marantz Henig, a contributing writer, has written recently for the magazine about the neurobiology of lying and about obesity.

Pueblos. Revista de información y debate.





ENTREVISTA. Mike Davis.

Mike Davis: “Los suburbios de las ciudades del tercer mundo son el nuevo escenario geopolítico decisivo” Mike Davis: “Los suburbios de las ciudades del tercer mundo son el nuevo escenario geopolítico decisivo”

En pocos años, por primera vez en la historia de la humanidad, la población urbana superará en número a la rural. Ahora bien, la mayor parte de estos urbanitas no vive en lo que normalmente entendemos por ciudad, sino en inmensos suburbios sin apenas infraestructuras ni servicios que escapan a cualquier conceptualización tradicional. Mike Davis, uno de los pensadores más recomendables de los últimos años y una fuente inagotable de nuevas perspectivas y temas de estudio -próximamente publicará una breve historia del coche bomba-, aborda esta nueva realidad en Planet of Slums (Planeta de Suburbios), uno de esos libros imprescindibles que te hacen preguntarte cómo es posible que no lo haya escrito nadie antes. Entre tanto, Traficantes de Sueños publica en español Ciudades muertas.

BLDGBLOG | 1 03 2007

En tu descripción de la nueva “geografía posturbana” utilizas un vocabulario novedoso: corredores regionales, conurbaciones difusas, redes policéntricas, periurbanización...

Se trata de un lenguaje en pleno proceso de desarrollo y en el que apenas reina el consenso. Los debates más interesantes han surgido a partir del estudio de la urbanización en el sur de China, Indonesia y el sudeste de Asia y giran principalmente en torno a la naturaleza de la periurbanización en la periferia de las grandes ciudades del tercer mundo. Con este término me refiero al lugar en el que se encuentran el campo y la ciudad y la pregunta que se plantea es: ¿estamos ante una fase temporal de un proceso complejo y dinámico o esta naturaleza híbrida se mantendrá a lo largo del tiempo?

La nueva realidad periurbana presenta una mezcla muy compleja de suburbios pobres, desplazados del centro de las ciudades y, entre medias, pequeños enclaves de clase media, frecuentemente de nueva construcción y vallados. En esta periurbanización encontramos también trabajadores rurales atrapados por la manufactura de baja remuneración y residentes urbanos que se desplazan diariamente para trabajar en la industria agrícola. Curiosamente, este fenómeno ha despertado también el interés de los analistas militares del Pentágono, que consideran estas periferias laberínticas uno de los grandes retos que deparará el futuro a las tecnologías bélicas y a los proyectos imperiales. Tras una época en la que se centraron en el estudio de los métodos de gestión empresarial de moda -el just-in-time y el modelo Wal Mart-, en el Pentágono parecen ahora obsesionados con la arquitectura y el planeamiento urbano. EE UU ha desarrollado una gran capacidad para destruir los sistemas urbanos clásicos, pero no tiene ningún éxito en las “Sader Cities” del mundo. El caso de Faluya es sintomático: después de que la destrozaran con bulldozers y bombas de racimo, los mismos insurgentes con los que se quiso acabar la reocuparon cuando acabó la ofensiva. Creo que la izquierda y la derecha coinciden en que los suburbios de las ciudades del tercer mundo son el nuevo escenario geopolítico decisivo.

¿Cuál es la representación cultural más adecuada de los suburbios del tercer mundo que describes en Planet of Slums?

Si Blade Runner fue un día el icono del futuro urbano, el Blade Runner de los suburbios es Black Hawk derribado. Reconozco que no puedo dejar de verla: su puesta en escena y su coreografía son increíbles. La película representa a la perfección esta nueva frontera de la civilización: la “misión del hombre blanco” en los suburbios del tercer mundo y sus amenazantes ejércitos con aspecto de videojuego enfrentándose a heroicos tecnoguerreros y a los rangers de la Delta Force. Por supuesto, desde el punto de vista moral es una película aterradora: es como un videojuego, en el que es imposible contar a todos los somalíes que mueren.

Por lo demás, la realidad es que los blancos no son mayoría entre los Rangers desplazados al extranjero: son americanos, sí, pero casi todos ellos proceden también de los suburbios. El nuevo imperialismo, como el viejo, tiene esta ventaja: la metrópoli es tan violenta y alberga tanta pobreza concentrada que produce excelentes guerreros para este tipo de campañas militares. Un antiguo profesor mío escribió un libro magnífico que mostraba, contra todo pronóstico, que en las victorias en las campañas militares del Imperio Británico el factor decisivo no era la tecnología armamentística sino la habilidad de los soldados británicos en el cuerpo a cuerpo con bayoneta, una habilidad que era consecuencia directa de la brutalidad de la vida cotidiana en los barrios bajos ingleses.

Más allá del giro hacia la violencia y la insurgencia, ¿está surgiendo algún sistema de autogobierno en los suburbios?

La organización en los suburbios es extraordinariamente diversa. En una misma ciudad latinoamericana, por ejemplo, hay desde iglesias pentecostales hasta Sendero Luminoso, pasando por organizaciones reformistas y ONG neoliberales. La popularidad de unos y otros colectivos varía muy rápidamente y es muy difícil hallar una tendencia general. Lo que está claro es que en la última década los pobres -y me refiero no sólo a los de los barrios urbanos clásicos que mostraban ya niveles altos de organización, sino también a los nuevos pobres de las periferias- se han estado organizando a gran escala, ya sea en una ciudad iraquí como Sader City o en Buenos Aires. Los movimientos sociales organizados han puesto sobre la mesa reivindicaciones de participación política y económica sin precedentes, que han impulsado un avance en la democracia formal. Sin embargo, generalmente los votos tienen poca relevancia: los sistemas fiscales del tercer mundo son, con escasas excepciones, tan regresivos y corruptos y disponen de tan pocos recursos que es casi imposible poner en marcha una redistribución real. Además, incluso en aquellas ciudades en las que hay un mayor grado de participación en las elecciones, el poder real se transfiere a agencias ejecutivas, autoridades industriales y entidades de desarrollo de todo tipo sobre las que los ciudadanos no tienen ningún control y que tienden a ser meros vehículos locales de las inversiones del Banco Mundial. La vía democrática hacia el control de las ciudades -y, sobre todo, de los recursos necesarios para acometer las reformas urbanas- sigue siendo increíblemente difícil.

En casi todos los programas gubernamentales o estatales que intentan abordar la pobreza urbana, el suburbio pobre se entiende como un mero subproducto de la superpoblación.

No tengo ninguna confianza en el concepto de superpoblación. La cuestión fundamental no es si la población ha aumentado demasiado, sino cómo cuadrar el círculo entre, por un lado, la justicia social y el derecho a un nivel de vida decente y, por otro lado, la sostenibilidad ambiental. No hay demasiada gente en el mundo; lo que sí hay, obviamente, es un sobreconsumo de recursos no renovables. Por supuesto, la solución ha de pasar por la propia ciudad: las ciudades verdaderamente urbanas son los sistemas más eficientes ambientalmente hablando que hemos creado para la vida en común. Ofrecen altos niveles de vida a través del espacio y el lujo públicos, lo que permite satisfacer necesidades que el modelo de consumo privado suburbano no puede permitirse. Dicho esto, el problema básico de la urbanización mundial actual es que no tiene nada que ver con el urbanismo clásico. El auténtico desafío es conseguir que la ciudad sea mejor como ciudad. Planet of Slums da la razón a los sociólogos que señalaron en los años cincuenta y sesenta los problemas de la suburbanización norteamericana: ocupación caótica del territorio, incremento de los tiempos de traslado del domicilio al trabajo y de los recursos asociados a este traslado, deterioro de la calidad del aire y falta de equipamientos urbanos clásicos.

Pero, ¿acaso no hay ciudades demasiado pobladas para el entorno tan escaso en recursos en el que están implantadas?

La inviabilidad de una megaciudad tiene menos que ver con el número de personas que viven en ella que con su modo de consumir: si se reutilizan y reciclan recursos y si se comparte el espacio público, entonces es viable. Hay que tener en cuenta que la huella ecológica varía muchísimo según los grupos sociales. En California, por ejemplo, el ala derecha de los movimientos conservacionistas sostiene que hay una enorme marea de inmigrantes mexicanos que es la responsable de la congestión y la polución, lo cual es completamente absurdo: no hay población con menor huella ecológica o que tienda a utilizar el espacio público de forma más intensa que los inmigrantes de Latinoamérica. El auténtico problema son los blancos que se pasean en sus cochecitos de golf por los ciento diez campos que hay en Coachella Valley. En otras palabras, un hombre de mi edad ocioso puede estar usando diez, veinte o treinta veces más recursos que una chicana que intenta salir adelante con su familia en un apartamento del centro de la ciudad.

No hay que dejarse llevar por el pánico al crecimiento de la población o a la llegada de inmigrantes; lo que hay que hacer es pensar cómo se pueden fomentar las aptitudes del urbanismo para lograr, por ejemplo, que suburbios como los de Los Ángeles funcionen como una ciudad en el sentido clásico. También hay que respetar la necesidad absoluta de conservar las zonas verdes y las reservas ambientales sin las cuales las ciudades no pueden funcionar. La tendencia actual en todo el mundo es que los pobres busquen acomodo en zonas húmedas de importancia vital, que se instalen en espacios abiertos cruciales para el metabolismo de la ciudad. Ahí está el ejemplo de Bombay, donde los más pobres se han asentado en un Parque Nacional adyacente donde, de cuando en cuando, se los comen los leopardos, o de São Paulo, donde se emplean enormes cantidades de sustancias químicas para purificar el agua porque se está librando una batalla perdida contra la polución en la cabecera de sus fuentes de abastecimiento. Si se permite este tipo de crecimiento, si se pierden las zonas verdes y los espacios abiertos, si los acuíferos se bombean hasta agotarlos y se contaminan los ríos, se daña fatalmente la ecología de la ciudad.

Ahora es el tiempo

Martin Luther King "I have a dream"

El cartel

http://www.geocities.com/SoHo/Studios/8016/cartel30.gif

http://www.geocities.com/SoHo/Studios/8016/cartel12.gif
El cartel es "un periódico mural de opinión". Aproximadamente cada dos meses, estos ilustradores de prensa tan conocida como El Mundo, Generación siglo XXI, 20 Minutos, El Siglo y colaboradores de fanzines como Mundo bruto, El cretino o Qué suerte, se reúnen en torno a una botella de ron de la Martinica, por ejemplo, y deciden un tema. Dividen el papel en cuatro partes, cada uno expresa su idea y cada uno aporta un icono para la cabecera. Hasta ahora, el cartel se ha impreso en una sola tinta y cada diez números, en dos.


http://www.geocities.com/SoHo/Studios/8016/cartel16.gif

Le Monde diplomatique

Domingo 04 de Marzo de 107



Marzo 2007. Numero 137

Vuelos secretos de la CIA

Por IGNACIO RAMONET


PORTADA LE MONDE DIPLOMATIQUE, EDICIÓN ESPAÑOLA Nº 137 MARZO DE 2007¿Indecencia? ¿Cinismo? ¿Perversión? ¿Cómo calificar la actitud de los Gobiernos europeos, sorprendidos en flagrante delito de complicidad con servicios de inteligencia extranjeros en el secuestro clandestino de decenas de sospechosos arrastrados hacia prisiones secretas y entregados a la tortura? ¿Podemos imaginar más flagrante violación de los derechos de la persona humana, cometida por Estados que no dejan de pregonar su apego a la ley?

Dos acontecimientos recientes son testimonio de la esquizofrenia reinante. En primer lugar, el 7 de febrero último en París, la solemne firma, por la mayor parte de los Gobiernos europeos, de la Convención de la Organización de las Naciones Unidas (ONU) contra las "desapariciones forzadas" (1), que criminaliza el uso de las prisiones secretas. Y luego, el 14 de febrero, en el Parlamento Europeo de Estrasburgo, la aprobación de un informe que acusa a esos mismos Gobiernos de complicidad con la Central Intelligence Agency (CIA) estadounidense, en operaciones de secuestros clandestinos.


Según ese informe (2), entre 2001 y 2005, los aviones de la CIA hicieron no menos de 1.245 escalas en aeropuertos europeos, llevando con frecuencia a bordo a sospechosos víctimas de "desapariciones forzadas", conducidos clandestinamente hacia la cárcel ilegal de Guantánamo o hacia prisiones de países cómplices (Egipto o Marruecos), donde la tortura es una práctica habitual.
Desde entonces es evidente que los Gobiernos europeos no ignoran nada acerca de la naturaleza criminal de esos vuelos secretos. Por otra parte, algunos de ellos no se limitaron solamente a cerrar los ojos. Polonia y Rumanía, por ejemplo, son particularmente sospechosos de haber organizado en su territorio "pequeños Guantánamos" donde eran encarceladas, a la espera de su traslado definitivo, personas secuestradas en Pakistán, Afganistán u otros lugares.
Se presume que el Gobierno británico participó en el secuestro de sospechosos y en su maltrato. Lo mismo que los Gobiernos sueco y austríaco. En cuanto a las autoridades alemanas, se las acusa, entre otras cosas, de "no haber ignorado" el secuestro de uno de sus residentes, de origen libanés, Jaled El-Masri, que fue trasladado a Afganistán y torturado. Los servicios secretos italianos, por su parte, están acusados de haber ayudado a agentes de la CIA a secuestrar clandestinamente en Milán al imán Osama Mustafá Hasan Naser, llamado "Abu Omar", y de trasladarlo a Egipto, a un calabozo de la prisión Al Tora, en el sur de El Cairo, donde habría de ser torturado y violado (3).
Queda claro que esta masiva violación de los derechos humanos no pudo perpetrarse sin que los servicios del Representante de la Política Exterior y de Seguridad Común de la Unión Europea, Javier Solana, así como los de su colaborador, el coordinador europeo de la lucha antiterrorista, Giis de Vries, hayan tenido conocimiento. De Vries, en un gesto elocuente, optó por renunciar a su cargo: "Los Estados democráticos -advirtió- deben librar su combate antiterrorista en el marco del respeto de las leyes (...). La acumulación de malos tratos en Abu Ghraib, los abusos de Guantánamo y los secuestros de la CIA han socavado la credibilidad de Estados Unidos y de Europa" (4).
Dirigentes o ejecutores, todos aquellos que han participado en esos secuestros deben temer a la justicia. Y meditar sobre el destino de María Estela Martínez, llamada "Isabelita Perón", ex presidenta de Argentina, un país donde en nombre del antiterrorismo las autoridades practicaron masivamente secuestros políticos. Acaba de ser detenida en Madrid, acusada de la "desaparición forzada" del estudiante Héctor Faguetti, en febrero de 1976, hace treinta y un años... La justicia es lenta, pero debe ser inexorable.



Notas:
(1) La Convención Internacional para la Protección de las Personas contra las Desapariciones Forzadas, fue firmada por alrededor de sesenta países, entre ellos Chile, Argentina y Uruguay, pero no Estados Unidos. Al menos veinte Estados tendrán que ratificarla para que entre en vigencia.
(2) www.europarl.europa.eu
(3) El pasado 16 de febrero, un tribunal de Milán inició un procedimiento judicial contra veintiséis agentes estadounidenses de la CIA y seis miembros de los servicios secretos italianos acusados de haber organizado en febrero de 2003 la "desaparición forzada" del imán Abu Omar.
(4) El País , Madrid, 17 de febrero de 2007.

Bitniks (revista multimedia)

Entrevista a Ignacio Ramonet

[ Director de Le Monde Diplomatique]

Ignacio Ramonet (Pontevedra 1943) es uno de los protagonistas del actual movimiento anti globalización. Desde la dirección de Le Monde Diplomatique (una publicación sobre política internacional que se distribuye en 10 lenguas a un millón de suscriptores) ha impulsado la creación del Forum Social Mundial. Esta organización, que se presentó en Porto Alegre Brasil en enero del 2001, tiene como objetivo agrupar a los movimientos que se oponen al "neoliberalismo, al dominio del mundo por el capital y a cualquier forma de capitalismo". Para Ramonet, el poder real reside hoy en el "poder mediático y en el financiero" y afirma que ahora nos enfrentamos a "`big brother´, un poder que ejerce un delicioso despotismo gracias a la publicidad".

¿Quién detenta hoy el poder real?

El poder financiero y el poder mediático que cada día están arrinconando más al poder político. El poder mediático difunde la información, la comunicación y la cultura lo que le convierte en el aparato ideológico que difunde los mensajes que llegan al gran público.


Las elecciones italianas del 13 de mayo son claves en este sentido. Silvio Berlusconi el hombre que tiene el poder mediático (domina el mayor conglomerado privado de medios de comunicación) y el financiero (es el más rico del país) aspira a conseguir el poder político de Italia.

¿Nos enfrentamos a un "enemigo de cara sonriente", como vaticinaba ya Aldoux Huxley en los años 30?

Estabamos acostumbrados a enfrentarnos a un enemigo áspero, pero ahora tenemos que enfrentarnos a "big brother", a un poder que ejerce un delicioso despotismo gracias a la publicidad. La publicidad nos presenta un mundo futuro idílico y paradisiaco en el que la gente vive feliz y eufórica. Aquellos que dan un mensaje diferente son tomados por agoreros.

¿Hay alternativa al proyecto globalizador de las multinacionales?

Por supuesto que sí y está alternativa se presentó en Porto Alegre el pasado mes de enero, en la reunión del Forum Social Mundial. Le Monde Diplomatique fuimos, como dicen los sudaméricanos, los "ideadores" de esta reunión. En este foro se presentaron propuestas concretas para superar el actual estado de cosas y avanzar hacia una sociedad que se centre en el ser humano.

Recientemente las multinacionales farmaceúticas han retirado su demanda contra la ley del Gobierno de Sudáfrica que autorizaba la venta de medicamentos genéricos, ¿Es esta la primera gran victoria del movimiento anti-globalización?

La decisión de las farmaceúticas de abandonar su demanda es de sentido común. Era escandaloso que negasen a la población de este país el derecho a la vida basándose en un absurdo derecho de patentes. Su actitud les estaba poniendo en contra de la opinión pública mundial, pero no ha sido la gran victoria. Ya se consiguió en su día derogar el AMI (Acuerdo Multilateral de Inversiones) y en Seattle se consiguió acabar con el proyecto de intentar convertir la cultura (el cine, los libros, los discos) en una mercancía más.

Internet no goza de sus simpatías ¿por qué?

Eso no es cierto. Le Monde Diplomatique fue el primer periódico de Francia en entrar en la red en 1994. En mi opinión Internet es una poderosa herramienta para acceder al conocimiento, el saber y para que la gente intercambie opiniones en los chats. Lo que pasa es que nosotros como periodistas hemos señalado alguno de los problemas que tiene Internet. Como dice, Paul Virilio "Los trenes inventaron el accidente de tren". Estamos a favor de Internet pero no participamos de la Neobeatitud de que La Red va a solucionarlo todo. Esa postura siempre nos ha parecido muy ingenua.


¿Qué opina del desplome de la burbuja financiera que se había creado en torno a los negocios de relacionados con las Nuevas Tecnologías?

En la película de Marco Ferreri "Break-up" (1966) el protagonista, una persona que se aburre por las noches cuando llega a casa, se dedica a inflar globos. Le gusta inflar los globos hasta el máximo, hasta el punto en que si se introduce algo más de aire explotarían. Esto es lo que hacen los especuladores en la Bolsa. Invierten en la burbuja hasta que calculan que no va a aguantar más y entonces se retiran. American On Line multiplicó su valor en bolsa por 800 desde su estreno en el parqué en 1992. Si su valor se multiplica por 800 por qué no por 900 o por 1000. ¿Dónde está el límite? Es todo un juego sin sentido.

Según el actual valor en Bolsa de AOL Time Warnet, ¿ no le parece descabellada la idea de que AOL absorbiese a Time Warner? ¿no debería haber sido al revés?

Estoy de acuerdo. Es descabellado que una compañía compre a otra con más tamaño, beneficios y empleados. Toda la operación se basaba en la valoración que los mercados bursátiles daban a AOL, un parámetro que ahora tras la caída del Nasdaq se ha visto que era totalmente absurdo.

Hiperpolítica.

Hae unos meses apareció un fenómeno llamado Delfín. Ésta es la historia. Bienvenidos al siglo XXI, con Delfín ¡hasta el fín!

Torres gemelas. Delfín.


Porqué me llamo Delfín.


Porqué Torres Gemelas.


Delfín hasta el fin, en Valparaiso.
"Nada representa mejor a los seres destrozados que un montón de añicos".
Rainer Maria Rilke

11-S. La caída del imperio.

URGENTE
Loose Change 2nd edition -vía sendero del peje-

Los pasos del Sendero

El blog, Sendero del peje, inaugura sus foros de discusión.

a darle.


http://toliro.lunarpages.com/foro/

c a r a a c a r a

"Miembros del proyecto artístico Cara a cara pegan retratos que muestran a personas israelíes y palestinas, en la barrera de Cisjordania, en Bethlehem. La obra realizada en el muro, que separa a ambas naciones, pretende generar conciencia y un cese en las hostilidades que se viven entre estos países en Medio Oriente."







La jornada

Nombre del distrito financiero de la ciudad de Nueva York, cuya bolsa de valores cerró este viernes con pérdidas

Bajo la Lupa

Alfredo Jalife-Rahme

Desglobalización: ¿el ser humano por encima del mercado?

Las potencias vencedoras imponen el modelo económico-financiero que más conviene a sus intereses. La unipolaridad de Estados Unidos, triunfadora de la guerra fría, impuso en 1991, fecha de la disolución de la Unión Soviética, sin enemigo al frente, el modelo que más le asienta y expande sus intereses: la globalización financiera, caracterizada por la desregulación, los paraísos fiscales, la contabilidad invisible y la burbuja de los "derivados" de alto riesgo.

La derrota geoestratégica de Estados Unidos en Irak quita la cobertura nuclear a la globalización financiera anglosajona, lo que se profundiza con la devaluación del dólar y su debilitamiento como moneda de reserva unipolar, y la probable recesión de su economía, lo que se complica con sus déficit consuetudinarios, sus múltiples burbujas especulativas y su deuda impagable.

El poder de la globalización financiera, controlada por el eje anglosajón de Wall Street y la City, se derrite a pasos acelerados y en cuyo seno será más dramático el fenómeno correctivo de la desglobalización que tendrá que deshacerse de todos sus variados cuan exóticos instrumentos especulativos (en su mayoría de existencia "virtual"), si es que no estallan antes en los manipulados "mercados".

Suena sumamente difícil que Washington pueda revertir su derretimiento del liderazgo de la globalización financiera, su parte más vulnerable. Su poderío militar unipolar de ensueño tecnológico ha sido detenido en forma increíble por la "guerra asimétrica" de dos insurgencias neomedievales. Dos entidades de la globalización financiera, Estados Unidos e Israel, acaban de sufrir dos severas derrotas a manos de la guerrilla sunita de Irak y del Hezbollah chiíta libanés, respectivamente.

Las dos derrotas del militarismo neoliberal global denotan nítidos alcances geoestratégicos que aceleran la desglobalización y trastocan en forma dramática la correlación planetaria de fuerzas que se encamina a la multipolaridad que por necesidad optará por un nuevo modelo tanto económico como financiero.

Pocos, pero muy sólidos, intelectuales del mayor nivel, como John Ralston Saul, se han atrevido a confrontar a la hidra de la globalización, lo que constituye una herejía en el mundo occidental carente de crítica cartesiana y de dialéctica creativa. El filósofo canadiense Ralston, exitoso emprendedor (montó la empresa estatal Petro-Canada), oriundo de un país de la anglosfera beneficiado por la globalización en sus variantes financiera, económica y petrolera, en su reciente libro, El colapso de la globalización y la reinvención del mundo (2005), opera la autopsia de la "desregulación" mediante una visión luminosa de más de 30 años. Refiere que, lejos de ser una fuerza inevitable, que ya se encuentra bajo el feroz ataque de las fuerzas nacionalistas, la globalización -una "ideología monolítica" de la "idolatría del mercado" con ínfulas tecnocráticas e imbuida de determinismo tecnológico-, pretendió suplantar a los estados-nación y permitió la paralizante acumulación de la deuda en el tercer mundo que ha reaccionado con la expansión de los movimientos contestatarios en el planeta. La globalización, que acabó en la desilusión de su utopía, se está pulverizando en numerosas piezas contradictorias, mientras los ciudadanos reafirman sus intereses nacionales". Ralston fustiga la pretensión de las trasnacionales de intentar sustituir la infraestructura de los gobiernos al confundir el "manejo gerencial" con el liderazgo carismático. Expone en forma persuasiva el retorno del nacionalismo tanto económico como político frente a las trasnacionales que socavaron los cimientos del estado-nación.

En efecto, en el mismo centro de la globalización, Estados Unidos, se ha generado un paulatino ajuste significativo hacia el neoproteccionismo, el neoaislacionismo, y la silenciosa "rerregulación" que forman parte de sus oscilaciones pendulares de apertura y cerrazón desde su génesis como nación.

Bajo el precepto de "patriotismo económico", que oculta su neoproteccionismo, Estados Unidos impidió que China comprara en dinero sonante a la petrolera Unocal que fue adquirida con papel chatarra por Chevron-Texaco. La misma tendencia neoproteccionista, se escenifica en países de la Unión Europea (UE) para impedir la "captura hostil" de empresas nacionales estratégicas por algunos de sus miembros. En Francia, otro pilar de la globalización, le llaman "nacionalismo económico".

El declive estadunidense es más aparentemente pronunciado en el subtipo de la "globalización económica", donde ha sido desplazado por sus competidores geoeconómicos de UE y el noreste asiático, donde despunta como nadie el "circuito étnico chino" (China, Hong Kong, Taiwán y Singapur) que, incluso, ha dejado atrás a Japón en todos los subtipos de la globalización.

En el ámbito de la "globalización económica", de lejos menos nociva que la "globalización financiera" extraviada en la especulación, existe un relativo empate entre los tres polos de poder regional del planeta que han acaparado sus beneficios: Estados Unidos, la UE y el noreste asiático (China, Japón y Sudcorea), a los que habría que agregar como nuevos actores a las "potencias emergentes" de Rusia, India y Brasil. La tendencia en el mediano plazo favorece al BRIC -Brasil, Rusia, India y China-, en detrimento de Estados Unidos y la UE.

Desde el punto de vista ideológico, el daño que asestó la globalización ha sido doble: conformó un "pensamiento único", como lo califica Ignacio Ramonet, editor de Le Monde Diplomatique, y castró, cuando no descerebró, a sus intelectuales. El caso de México con Salinas, el firmante del TLCAN con Daddy Bush, es patéticamente trágico, al haber desmantelado y comprado (literalmente) a los pocos que pensaban en forma independiente.

"Occidente" perdió la savia de la crítica que lo había impulsado a su constante autocorrección. Los intelectuales, por antonomasia "críticos" (un seudointelectual apadrinado y/o cobijado por el poder es un amanuense y/o palafrenero), simbolizan las neuronas del cerebro que preside las actividades del género humano, indaga los asuntos planetarios y crea las mejores opciones para los seres vivientes de la creación en la biosfera. ¿Cómo puede funcionar un cerebro sin neuronas? Esta quizá fue la mayor perdición del modelo globalizador: haber pretendido a la automatización robótica del pensamiento neoliberal mediante ecuaciones econométricas muy falibles, a las que siempre faltó una constante que pretendió trasmutar en una vulgar variable: el ser humano, la base fija de todas la ecuaciones habidas y por haber mientras perviva en el vasto universo.

Bajo el modelo de la globalización, en todos sus subtipos (financiera, económica y petrolera), la entelequia llamada "mercado", a imagen y semejanza de la aparatosa desigualdad distributiva de la riqueza en el planeta controlada por la plutocracia oligopólica, avasalló al género humano en su conjunto.

Mediante la desglobalización, el género humano tiene la oportunidad dorada de volver a controlar y regular el "mercado" de la neofeudal plutocracia oligopólica.

Los efectos psicológicos

El pelele, por fin abre la quijadita para referirse a las andanadas de Fox y sus yunketos en la sombra, y cual muñeca fea, --acostumbrada a que la avienten, polveen y arrinconen--, declaró: “(Vicente Fox) se ha conducido con respeto hacia mi persona y hacia los mexicanos, lo cual aprecio”. ¿Respeto? Esta subordinación delirante, ya no solo preocupa, sino ofende -y da pena-, ya que el titular del ejecutivo, si tuviera una pizca de independencia y legitimidad, no debería andarle limpiando las botas a su jefe máximo, al que -según las declaraciones del michoacano-, le tiene “un profundo respeto y aprecio”.

¿Profundo respeto y aprecio?

Naranjo. El cliente.

Contra(comunicado):

Como decía Henry David Thoreau, "No pido inmediatamente que no haya gobierno, sino inmediatamente un gobierno mejor". El orígen de Medios y política fue el fraude electoral del 2006: nació La República de la Televisión y la programa(ción) se volvió dicta(dura): un monopolio opinativo de Tercer Grado. Aquí en 'Medios y política' están las evidencias comunicacionales que sostienen nuestra tésis: Felipe Calderón no ganó las elecciones; la oligarquía lo impuso mediante un fraude para auto(comprarse) lo que queda de México. Y lo repitieron imponiendo a Enrique Peña Nieto en el 2012. Por eso pedimos lo posible: que se restaure La República.

Vistas a la página totales

Huracán: La política secreta neoliberal

Huracán: "Ayotzinapa. El motivo"

Powered By Blogger

Archivo del Blog